Nonverbal Learning Disabilities, Asperger’s Syndrome, Pervasive Developmental Disorder - Should We Care?
by Roger A. Brumback, MD; Caryn R. Harper, MS; Warren A. Weinberg, MD
www.nldline.com

Words are often considered the feature that differentiates humans from all other animals. It is clear that no other animals have the same anatomic-physiologic structures used by humans to create words. Anthropologists have long looked for archaelogic evidence of words as the beginning of humanness. The earliest evidence of written words - symbolic representation of spoken ideas - dates from about 10,000 BC, although we have no real idea when spoken words began. Nonetheless, modern life seems to be linked to the ability to understand and communicate in words.

Parents and physicians consider one of the most important milestones in child development to be the first words. Development of ability to use spoken and written words (reading) are key goals in school, and testing and readings are based mostly on the ability to identify and use words appropriately.

Since the identification of aphasia by Paul Broca1 and subsequent explosion of interest in language disorders following the work of Norman Geschwind2 and colleagues,3 it has become evident that specialized areas of the brain, specifically left cerebral hemisphere structures, underlie the ability to understand and use words.

However, effective human communication and interaction requires more than simply words. Facial expression, posture, gaze, and body movement (gesture) are as important as words in communicating correct information to another individual.4 Not only should words be appropriate for the social context (proper pragmatic speech), but the facial expression, posture, gaze, gestures, and vocal quality (prosody) must also be congruent.

Much of nonverbal communication appears to be a normal function of the right cerebral hemisphere.4,5 Lesions of the right hemisphere produce a variety of problems in communication, evident as difficulties producing or recognizing the tone, pitch, and melody of speech and associated facial expression, posture, and body gestures.5-14 It is noteworthy that infarcts involving the right cerebral hemisphere are frequently ore disabling than similar size lesions of the left hemisphere.15 Despite the lack of words, individuals with expressive aphasia from left cerebral hemisphere lesions can often still be understood by family members, on the basis of situation-appropriate emotional vocalizations and gestures.

A variety of functions appear to be best performed by the right cerebral hemisphere (Table 1).12,18-21 Spatial analysis, sequencing, and object recognition, are all necessary for the proper interpretation of the facial and body movements used in communication. The right hemisphere mediates the attentiveness necessary to discriminate communications from background distractions. The right hemisphere maintains visual and auditory imagery that permits proper understanding of the surrounding world including appropriate recognition and interaction with animate and inanimate objects. Empathy (caring), wit, and vigilance (alertness) are other important functions of the right cerebral hemisphere.

Individuals with normal intelligence who have lifelong difficulties with the verbal functions of the left cerebral hemisphere, presumably on the basis of neurodevelopmental abnormality, are said to have learning disabilities. Specific learning disabilities have been Federally defined as "a disorder in one or more of the basic psychological processes involved in understanding or in using language, spoken or written, which may manifest itself in an imperfect ability to listen, think, speak, read, write, spell, or do mathematical calculations," [PL94-142 (1975) and Individuals with Disabilities Act (1990)] and the prototype learning disability is dyslexia (poor reading ability). Thus, learning disabilities officially involve problems mainly with word tasks.

However, a large number of individual have neuro-developmental abnormalities involving functions of the right cerebral hemisphere. While not a part of the Federal definition for learning disabilities, these problems have been variously termed right hemisphere learning disabilities, visual-spatial and graphomotor learning disabilities, or social-emotional learning disabilities.22-26 However, the preferable designation is nonverbal learning disabilities.27-30

Combinations of a variety of difficulties characterize the child with a nonverbal learning disability. Graphomotor, visual-spatial, organizational, and sequencing problems produce significant difficulties both in school and in other environments. However, social interaction can be especially impaired by the difficulties recognizing faces and expression, aberrant prosody and emotionality in speech and gesture, pragmatic abnormalities of speech (appropriateness of the speech in relation to the social situation), misinterpretation of the tactile stimuli associated with interpersonal interactions (such as a handshake), and difficulty understanding the sequencing or interrelationship of events (problems "viewing the world as a series of interconnected events"). Depending on the range of dysfunctions and degree of severity, a wide variety of individual syndromes can be included under the heading of nonverbal learning disabilities (Table2). 18,19,31

In this issue of the Journal of Child Neurology, Bonnet and Gao32 provide a brief review of Asperger’s syndrome.33-38 The characteristic features of this disorder involve impaired reciprocal social interactions, abnormal nonverbal communication (but normal verbal ability), and stereotyped behaviors. These impairments all represent abnormalities in right hemisphere functions. Asperger’s syndrome appears to be a severe form of nonverbal learning disability in which a basic problem is expressive hyperdysprosodia and hyperemotionality. Individuals with Asperger’s syndrome are uncomfortable in social settings because of the difficulty responding appropriately in interactions with other individuals, and they are rejected by others because their nonverbal communication is "out of context" in relation to their verbal communication. This discomfort and rejection produces isolation and a relatively restricted and stereotyped activity pattern. It is important to recognize that Asperger’s syndrome is one of a number of brain-based social-emotional disorders that are the result of neurodevelopmental abnormalities. As more such patients are identified and studied with newer imaging techniques, a better understanding of this neurodevelopmental abnormality will be possible.

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by contributions from the Caleb C. and Julia W. Dula Education and Charitable Foundation, Mr. and Mrs. Woody Hunt (Cimmaron Foundation), and Mr. and Mrs. Morton Meyerson, and the Azoulay family.

References
  1. Broca P. Remarques sur le seige de la faculté du langage articulé, suives d’ume observation d’aphemie. Bulletins-Societé de Paris 1861; 2:330-357
  2. Geshwind N.: Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain 196;88:237-294, 585-644.
  3. Benson DF: The history of behavioral neurology. Neurol Clin 1993;11:1-8.
  4. Ross ED: Nonverbal aspects of language. Neurol Clin 1993;11:9-23.
  5. Ross, ED: The aprosodia: Functional-anatomic organization of the affective components of language in the right hemisphere. Arch Neurol 1981;38:561-569.
  6. Borod JC, Andelman F, Obler LK, et al: Right hemisphere specialization for the identification of emotional words and sentences: Evidence from stroke patients. Neuropsychologia 1992;30:827-844.
  7. Brumback RA, Staton RD, Wilson H: Right cerebral hemisphere dysfunction. Arch Neurol 1984;41:248-250.
  8. George MS, Parekh PI, Rosinsky N, et al: Understanding emotional prosody activates right hemisphere regions. Arch Neurol 1996;53:665-670.
  9. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Speedie L, Coslett HB: Comprehension of affective and nonaffective prosody. Neurology 1984;34:917-921.
  10. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Valenstein E, Watson RT: The right hemisphere: Neuropsychological functions. J Neurosurg 1986;64:693-704.
  11. Heilman KM, Scholes R, Watson RT: Auditory affective agnosia: Disturbed comprehension of affective speech. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1975;38:69-72.
  12. Heilman KM, Scholes R, Watson RT: Auditory affective agnosia: Disturbed comprehension of affective speech. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1975;38:69-72.
  13. Ross ED, Harney JH, deLacoste-Utamsing C, Purdy PD: How the brain integrates affective and propositional language into a unified behavioral functions: Hypothesis based on clinicoanatomic evidence. Arch Neurol 1981;38:745-748.
  14. Ross ED, Homan RW, Buck R: Differential hemispheric lateralization of primary and social emotions, repression, and the subconscious. Neuropsychiartr Neuropshychol Behav Neurol 1994;7:1-19.
  15. Calvanio R, Lelvine D, Petrone P: Elements of cognitive rehabilitation after right hemisphere stroke. Neurol Clin 1993;11:25-27.
  16. Nass RD: Sex differences in learning abilities and disabilities. Ann Dyslexia 1993;43:62-77.
  17. Geschwind N Galaburda AM: Cerebral lateralization: biologic mechanisms, associations and pathology. Arch Neurol 1985;42:428-459,521-552,634-654.
  18. Weinberg WA, Harper CR, Brumback RA: Use of the symbol language and communication battery in the physician’s office for assessment of higher brain functions. J Child Neurol 1995;10(Suppl 1):s23-s31.
  19. Weinberg WA Harper CR, Brumback RA: Neuroanatomic substrate of developmental specific learning disabilities and select behavioral syndromes. J Child Neurol 1995;10(Suppl 1):s78-s81.
  20. Molfese DL, Segalowitz SJ (eds): Brain Lateralization in Children: Developmental Implications. New York, Guilford Press, 1988.
  21. Brumback RA, Weinberg WA: Pediatric behavioral neurology: An update on the neurologic aspects of depression, hyperactivity, and learning disabilities. Neurol Clin 1990;8:677-703.
  22. Deuell RK: Developmental dysgraphia and motor skill disorders. J Child Neurol 1995;10(Suppl 1):s16-s22.
  23. Denckla, M: The neuropsychology of social-emotional learning disabilities. Arch Neurol 1983;40:461-462.
  24. Voeller KKS: Right-hemisphere deficit syndrome in children. Am J Psychiatry 1986;143:1004-1009.
  25. Voeller KKS: Clinical neurologic aspects of right-hemisphere deficit syndrome. J Child Neurol 1995;10(Suppl 1):s16-s22.
  26. Weintraub S, Mesulam M-M: Developmental learning disabilities of the right hemisphere. Emotional, interpersonal and cognitive components. Arch Neurol 1983;40:463-468.
  27. Myklebust HR: Nonverbal learning disabilities: Assessment and intervention, in Myklebust HR (ed): Progress in Learning Disabilities, vol 3. New York, Grune & Stratton, 1975,pp 85-121.
  28. Rourke BP: Nonverbal Learning Disabilities: The Syndrome and the model. New York, Guilford Press, 1989.
  29. Rourke BP:(ed): Syndrome of Nonverbal Learning Disabilities: Neurodevelopmental Manifestations. New York, Guilford Press, 1995.
  30. Badian NA: Nonverbal learning disability, school behavior, and dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia 1992;42:159-178.
  31. Geschwind N: The brain of a learning-disabled individual. Ann Dyslexia 1984;34:319-327.
  32. Bonnet KA, Gao X-K: Asperger’s syndrome in Neurologic Perspective. J Child Neurol 1996;11:483-489.
  33. Asperger H: Die "Autisichen Psychopathen" im Kindersalter. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkrankh 1944;117:76-136.
  34. Frith U(ed): Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 1991.
  35. Gilberg C: Asperger syndrome in 23 Swedish children. Dev Med Child Neurol 1989;31:520-531.
  36. Gilberg IC, Gilberg C: Asperger syndrome-some epidemiological considerations: A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 1989;30:631-638.
  37. Wing L: Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical account. Psychiatr Med 1981;121:115-130.
  38. Siegel B: The World of the Autistic Child: Understanding and Treating Autistic Spectrum Disorders. New York, Oxford University Press, 1996.

Printed with permission from the author. Address correspondence to Dr. Roger A. Brumback, OUHSC-BMSB 451, 940 Stanton L. Young Boulevard, Oklahoma City, OK 73104-5042.